Skip to main content

Language: English / Gàidhlig

Loading…
Chamber and committees

Finance Committee

Meeting date: Wednesday, March 7, 2012


Contents


Growing up in Scotland Study

The Convener

Item 3 is an evidence-taking session with the researchers and Scottish Government officials involved in the growing up in Scotland study. I welcome to the meeting Paul Bradshaw from ScotCen Social Research; Donna Bell and Wendy van Rijswijk from the Scottish Government; and Marco Biagi MSP, who is attending in his role as co-convener of the cross-party group on children and young people.

Mr Bradshaw, I understand that you would like to make a short opening statement.

Paul Bradshaw (ScotCen Social Research)

Indeed, convener.

Good morning, everyone. We are very glad to be here today and want to thank you for the invitation. The growing up in Scotland study—or GUS, as we more commonly refer to it—is a major longitudinal research project that tracks the lives of several cohorts of Scottish children through their early years and beyond. A longitudinal research project simply follows a discrete group of individuals over time. The individuals in such a study are usually linked in some way and, in the case of GUS, the children in each of the three groups who participate are linked by their dates of birth—they have all been born within the same 12-month period. We call the 3,000 children born in 2002-03 our child cohort, the 5,000 born in 2004-05 our first birth cohort and the 6,000 born in 2010-11 our second birth cohort. The size of our two birth cohorts means that around one in 10 children born in Scotland in the specified year have been included in the study.

We have been invited to give evidence as part of the committee’s on-going consideration of preventative spending, under which approach negative social outcomes are prevented rather than dealt with when they occur. Not only can data from GUS tell us about children’s outcomes but, because of its longitudinal design and focus on early years, the study is uniquely placed to provide understanding of the influence of children’s early circumstances and experiences on those outcomes. As members might have seen from our briefing paper or indeed any of the study’s reports, data from GUS has already provided significant insight to that end from a specifically Scottish perspective.

For example, our findings have explored the cognitive ability gap between children aged three and five from different social backgrounds, how that gap changes and the early experiences that influence change in cognitive ability during that time. The results demonstrate, for example, the importance of good early parent-child attachment and an active home learning environment from a young age in ensuring continuing positive cognitive development, particularly among children from more disadvantaged backgrounds.

The study is funded by the Scottish Government, so one of GUS’s key aims is to provide evidence to support the long-term monitoring and evaluation of policies for children and families, with a specific focus on early years. The study aims to produce an holistic view of the lives of children in Scotland; in other words, it seeks to provide information on the many and varied aspects of children’s lives that influence the different trajectories their lives might take. Such aspects include their family relationships and circumstances, their health, their neighbourhood and their childcare and school experiences. We have shared the GUS findings with a wide range of audiences via our considerable dissemination programme and, as a result, study findings have influenced a number of policy and practice developments related to, among other things, the delivery of antenatal care, parenting support programmes, childcare and early years education.

The study is now in its eighth year. The next set of findings will be published in May and those separate reports will address early experiences of primary school; grandparents’ involvement in children’s lives; and overweight, obesity and activity. We have also recently completed the first full year of data collection with our new birth cohort and expect to report on the findings in late 2012 or early 2013. Among other things, that report will provide a comparison of the circumstances of children born in 2004-05 and those of children born in 2010-11.

GUS is a unique resource, which provides a range of stakeholders with invaluable evidence about the experiences of and outcomes for children and families in Scotland. Although considerable analysis of the data has already been undertaken, there is still much that could be learned from what we have gathered, and its value continues to grow. With each round of data collection, it becomes possible to map further and more fully the varied and complex pathways that Scottish children take as they move through the early years into later childhood and beyond.

Of course, none of this would be possible without the on-going support and participation of the families and children involved, to whom we are extremely grateful. I hope that members have had a chance to consider our briefing paper and some of the reports from the study. We are happy to answer any questions.

Thank you very much.

I will open with a simple question. How did you choose the participants in the study?

Paul Bradshaw

The participants were chosen at random. We took what we call a stratified sampling approach and applied it to child benefit records. Essentially, we selected a period of 12 months; children who were born in that period were eligible for selection. We took data zones—small geographic areas at postcode level—and listed all the data zones for Scotland according to local authority and area deprivation. We selected about 130 areas at random, and all the children in those areas were invited to participate in the study.

The Convener

A few years ago, a lot of us were quite shocked by the main findings of the UNICEF report “Child poverty in perspective: An overview of child well-being in rich countries”, which showed that the UK was near the bottom and, in some cases, at the bottom of the rankings on five of the six dimensions of children’s quality of life that were reviewed. Has your study picked up good reasons for that? Why was that the case? How can we ensure that things change for the better in the years to come?

Paul Bradshaw

We are just at the stage of looking at that aspect of the data. We are about to launch the next round of fieldwork with our existing birth cohort. The children will be eight years old. We will ask them personally what they think about their lives. As part of that, we will attempt to measure their wellbeing. Once we have that data, we will be in a much stronger position to look at the factors that are present in children’s lives up to that point and how, for better or worse, those factors affect their perspective of their wellbeing. To date, that is not something that we have looked at specifically.

The Convener

Section 3 of your briefing paper deals with social, emotional and behavioural development. It points out that, by the time they start school,

“about 1 in 10 Scottish children have moderate or severe social, emotional or behavioural difficulties. This rises to around 1 in 4 in relation to difficulties with conduct.”

As well as noting that that is happening, have you looked at how we might be able to change things? Can you detail some of the changes that could be made to improve matters?

Paul Bradshaw

Certainly. The data that you referred to was measured at the point at which children were entering school, but we have earlier data on the same children, which was measured when they were around the age of three. We found that a large number of those children who exhibited difficulties on entering school also exhibited the same difficulties when they were three, which suggested that those children could be identified at an earlier stage. In addition, we found that certain aspects of their lives up to the age of three—in particular, aspects of their home environment, such as how they were parented and their material circumstances—were associated with poorer outcomes.

I suggest that some of the programmes that are already in place around improving parenting support will be helpful in improving social, emotional and behavioural outcomes for children. Improved universal services, whereby children come into contact with services more regularly at an earlier stage, might be helpful in identifying some of those issues earlier on. That would allow action to be taken or support to be delivered so that by the time the children get to school, many of those difficulties have been tackled or the children are at least being supported.

11:30

How are the GUS findings feeding into Scottish Government policy?

Donna Bell (Scottish Government)

We want to be clear that all the work that Paul Bradshaw and his colleagues have been doing on GUS is having a great impact on informing our thinking.

In response to your previous question about what we can do to minimise problems such as conduct disorder and the social, emotional and behavioural issues that children experience, Paul Bradshaw is absolutely right: in many cases those things can be detected long before children reach school age.

A few things have been put in place to address that. You all know about the getting it right for every child approach, which generally involves getting children the help that they need when they need it. If issues are identified in families pre-birth, we should be looking at providing the right amount of support for parents and children at birth.

There is additional work at present on the health visitor 24 to 30-month check, which is when one would begin to see children demonstrating speech and language delay and other developmental delay. At that point, it is important to be able to put in place clear responses to turn that around.

There is a lot of other work to support that. Paul Bradshaw mentioned attachment, which is critical for good development and in enabling children to form good social relationships. A range of activity is going on in that regard. Parenting is also crucial to children’s development. The parenting strategy is currently in development, and we are talking to around 100 organisations about what support parents think they need to be better parents.

Paul Bradshaw mentioned the effective programmes that address conduct disorder and social, emotional and behavioural issues. Those include parenting support programmes such as triple P—the positive parenting programme—and the incredible years programmes, which focus specifically on conduct disorder.

We are doing some work in the early years task force on which of those programmes are most effective and how we can work with the NHS, local government and so on to deliver them more consistently across the country.

I open up the discussion to committee members. We will have a question from Michael McMahon, followed by one from Mark McDonald.

Michael McMahon

Thank you, convener. I know how concerned you are about our diplomatic skills this morning, so I will preface my question by saying that, as a sociology graduate, I am aware that sociology has sometimes been described as complex explanations of the patently obvious. That does not mean that longitudinal studies such as GUS are not hugely important and do not have a huge impact in guiding policy, rather that they often reflect what one suspected might be the case in the first place.

Have the studies so far thrown up anything that you did not expect, or has the evidence that you have seen disabused you of any preconceived notions?

Paul Bradshaw

A lot of our findings tend to confirm what people thought already; we produce very robust Scottish evidence that those differences and associations exist.

On whether anything that has turned up in the results has surprised us, the key thing that always comes to mind when I am asked that question is how stark the differences are between children who suffer from multiple disadvantages and children who do not. We expected to find differences in outcomes for children, but we did not expect there to be such a range of them from day one, nor did we expect them to persist.

Has the study thrown up any of the structural reasons behind those differences? Are you concerned about the structural reasons for them, or do you just want to identify where the problem lies?

Paul Bradshaw

The design of the study and the data that we produce are better for identifying the factors that are associated with difficulties arising and poorer outcomes than for identifying structural issues that are preventing the achievement of better outcomes. We rely on those who are more involved in using our data to say what might be preventing something from happening or whether something could be changed to improve it.

Mark McDonald

Have you examined the impact that post-natal depression in the mother has on the child? It has an obvious impact on the mother, but is there evidence of its impact on the child when it is not detected early, or not treated, compared with its impact on the child when it is treated?

Paul Bradshaw

The answer is yes and no. We have looked at the influence of maternal mental health on child outcomes. A report was published at sweep 4—which would have been about May 2010—that used the first four waves of data to consider the influence of maternal mental health on social, emotional and behavioural outcomes for children. However, we did not specifically measure post-natal depression because the first contact that we had with families in the birth cohort was at 10 months, by which point it is too late to measure post-natal depression in a lot of mothers who suffer from it. Nevertheless, we measure maternal mental health on an on-going basis, and we have shown that poorer maternal mental health is associated with poorer early social, emotional and behavioural outcomes for children aged three and four.

More recently, we have produced research that looks at some of the events that appear to influence changes in maternal mental health. If we know what triggers a change in maternal mental health and we know that maternal mental ill health is associated with poorer behavioural outcomes in children, we can say that, if we prevent things such as job loss and couple separation from happening, we may prevent changes in maternal mental health, which may lead to better outcomes in children’s social, emotional and behavioural development.

Mark McDonald

Were the findings the same irrespective of background—whether the mother suffered disadvantage—or were they relative? For example, would a child from a less-advantaged background whose mother had maternal mental health issues suffer a greater disadvantage because of their background and that factor than a child from a more advantaged background whose mother had poor maternal mental health?

Paul Bradshaw

All our analysis in the reports that were published in sweep 3 used complex statistical procedures to allow us to control for the effect of background. Therefore, we are able to say that, irrespective of household income and the level of parental education, maternal mental health has an association with behavioural outcomes. Irrespective of whether someone lives in a high-income household or a low-income household, if their mother experiences poor mental health they are more likely to have poor behavioural outcomes.

Is the effect not accentuated? Are children in a less-advantaged situation not likely to suffer more as a result of poor maternal mental health than children in a higher-income household?

Donna Bell

I am not sure what the specific evidence will tell you, but it has emerged from the study and other work that we have done that the issue is not specifically about the impact of mental health but about parents’ ability or willingness to engage with services. Those who have a lower standard of education and fewer advantages are much less likely to engage with services, so they are less likely to get treatment. If the problem is not being treated, the impact on the children will be greater.

Mark McDonald

I was about to come on to that. Your report says that those from a less-advantaged background, or who have a lower standard of education, are less likely to access services. Birmingham City Council touched on that point when it came before the committee. How do we reach those people who are not necessarily unwilling but who do not access the services? Do the services have to make more of a proactive effort to find those people and get them involved? Are you looking at that?

Donna Bell

Absolutely. We are very keen to see the light touch of initiatives such as the play talk read bus going to areas in which few standard services are available, through to health visitors taking a more proactive approach. The third sector also plays an important role. Organisations such as Family Circle Care in Edinburgh and the Jeely Piece Club in Glasgow and a range of others throughout Scotland are engaging with people in their family life rather than waiting for them to come to a place. That is an important point.

Elaine Murray

Like the convener, I was a bit surprised to read that

“1 in 10 Scottish children have moderate or severe social, emotional or behavioural difficulties”

and that one in four have difficulties with conduct. What type of difficulties are you talking about? What implication does that statistic have for pre-school and primary school children?

Paul Bradshaw

We use an instrument called the strengths and difficulties questionnaire to measure social, emotional and behavioural development across a number of domains. At that age, a conduct problem simply means exhibiting bad behaviour, if you like. That is probably the easiest way to explain it.

What would you call bad behaviour in a child of four?

Paul Bradshaw

I cannot recall the specific items off the top of my head, but I could certainly point you in the right direction. We are talking about unruly behaviour beyond that which would be expected of a child aged four or five, and which would cause the parent to report it frequently. The instrument measures difficulties with peer relationships, such as children not finding it easy to get on with friends, or arguing with friends. It reports on emotional symptoms, which is the extent to which children have low self-esteem or no confidence. The instrument also measures hyperactivity and inattention, or the child’s ability to sit still or pay attention or follow through an action or task that they have been set.

Those are some of the difficulties that children might face, particularly in making the transition to a school environment in which they are expected to sit still for a lot of the time and to do things that are asked and expected of them. It is likely that they will be placed in an environment with children whom they have not met before and they will be required to establish relationships. They might be asked to perform tasks on their own or to speak out in front of the class at show-and-tell, which could be difficult for someone with low self-esteem or no confidence. Those are some of the immediate issues that children will face on entering primary school, which might make the transition negative. We know from other research that that transition can influence later school experiences and overall outcomes.

Elaine Murray

Could it not be argued that some of the expectations that we have of young children going to school are unrealistic? Other countries take a less rigid approach to the very early years so that slightly more exuberant behaviour is not regarded as disruptive.

11:45

Donna Bell

Curriculum for excellence looks to do things differently. The early phase up to age eight will be much more—I am reluctant to say play based, because the approach is much more structured than those words might infer—engaging for children and will place realistic expectations on them. We have moved on from the approach whereby children need to sit down and listen for long periods of time.

Elaine Murray

The study suggests that another negative factor is harsh punishment by parents. There is a view in society that children are badly behaved because they are not disciplined enough. That is perpetuated by the television shows that show Supernanny telling parents that they are too soft, sticking the kid on the naughty step and all the rest of it.

It is difficult for parents not to have the impression that their children are badly behaved because they are not strict enough. How do you tackle that issue and ensure that parents of very young children understand the types of restraint or punishment that are appropriate for a very young child?

Paul Bradshaw

That is very difficult. We were very cautious with that finding, because all that we have determined is that there is a relationship between harsh punishment and a greater likelihood of, for example, conduct difficulties or hyperactivity. We do not know in which direction that relationship flows. We do not know whether the harsh punishment is a parental reaction in response to a child who is more difficult to control, or whether the difficult behaviour occurs as a result of a particular style of punishment. That is clear in the report.

I know from my knowledge of the parenting literature that parenting programmes try to help parents find the ideal middle ground, whereby they are not too relaxed about how they approach the discipline of their children but their children are offered a certain degree of autonomy. The household and the parenting should not be chaotic and there should be rules so that children know what their boundaries are, but they should have some awareness of their independence within those boundaries. That is a challenge for those who deliver the programmes.

The Convener

Parental consistency is a key issue. Children need to know exactly where they are. You are right that they need firm boundaries, but they need to be able to breathe within those boundaries.

Elaine Murray touched on the point that perhaps, in the Scottish system, we have too many expectations of children. In Scandinavia and elsewhere children start school aged six or seven. I remember having to complain to my son’s primary school when, at the age of five and only one week into primary school, he was given a mountain of homework to do, even though he had been at school for only two days.

Some parents are pushy and seem to like that kind of thing, but there is an issue about children being allowed to develop and play when they come home from primary school rather than the school day being extended. Scottish parents sometimes want to send their kids to school when they are four and a half, never mind five. Is that an issue? In other countries, parents are much more relaxed about when they send their children to school.

Paul Bradshaw

The issue has come up a wee bit because the data that we are looking at now, ahead of the report that is due to be published in May, considers some issues about transition and the reasons that parents gave for deferring their kids’ entry to school. We have been slightly surprised by the findings, because most deferral decisions have been based on parental concerns about development. We expected that deferrals would be more common among more educated parents who would decide, “My child is too young to start school and they would benefit more from a later start.” In fact, most of the decisions are fuelled by real concerns about speech, behaviour or some sort of physical health issue that is pushing them back.

I have concluded that it is hard to tell from the literature on age at school entry what is best. I am not sure that we have unrealistic expectations of children, but there is probably some scope to prepare them better for the move to school. By offering something better in the pre-school experience, perhaps we can ensure that, by the time children enter school, they are more prepared to do so.

I welcome Gavin Brown back from his 35-minute natural break.

Apologies, convener.

Paul Wheelhouse is next.

Paul Wheelhouse

I will touch on a couple of issues. First, I will reel back a bit to the methodology of collecting the data and selecting the children who were involved in the study. You talked earlier about that and mentioned that 130 data zones throughout Scotland had been selected.

We have received a lot of evidence about how we will rely heavily on the community planning partnerships in the implementation of preventative spending. How readily could the data that you have collected be used to inform the decisions of the 32 community planning partnerships? Are there any barriers to using the data in that way? Were there any particular differences in the outcomes that were experienced in the study, particularly at a local level?

Paul Bradshaw

The question about local-level data is another one that we are often asked. The study was not designed to produce data at a local level, but we have argued about its value with those who are involved in collecting data for community planning partnerships. We have had a number of conversations with them about it, because we can demonstrate from our quite extensive data set the expected outcomes for children who live in an area with high deprivation and high unemployment and, perhaps, in a lone-parent household. We can also demonstrate the different behaviours, experiences and circumstances that would benefit such children.

Those factors apply irrespective of where someone lives. Whether in a deprived area in Fife or one in Aberdeen, children in families with that combination of factors would still benefit from the parenting behaviours and home learning environment that we identify as beneficial. That is the value of the study in assisting CPPs in developing interventions.

Paul Wheelhouse

I totally accept that and understand that that is what you set out to achieve. Is there any way that we can be a bit smarter about using the information that you collect to inform local decision makers about whether they are on track if, for example, there have been improvements in the samples that you have taken locally?

Perhaps you go to the same data zones. If the sampling is entirely random, the data zones might be different, which might make it a bit more difficult. However, if they match areas that are typical and representative of a geographical area and you are able to see whether there has been an improvement over time as the longitudinal nature of the study kicks in, that might help us to understand whether the measures that are being taken in an area are having the desired impact.

Paul Bradshaw

That would be difficult, because we do not have the numbers at the local level to demonstrate it. Obviously, we have children who live in all local authority areas but, in some of those areas, we may have only 60 or 70 families. We could pull out their data and say that, because families in Fife, for example, have been permitted to take advantage of a particular parenting programme since 2008, we will examine how their outcomes have changed. However, the statistical caveats that we would have to put on any such analysis would render it dangerous and mostly meaningless, so we have been reluctant to use the data in that manner.

However, there are some local-level possibilities. We release the data on selected health boards. If a particular parenting programme is being delivered in the Greater Glasgow and Clyde NHS Board area—which is the case, in fact—we could compare the outcomes of children in that area with the lives of children in the Lothians, for example. We can also make comparisons between children in some of the bigger health board areas and those in the rest of Scotland.

Alternatively, if local authorities have sufficient resources to collect their own data, and if they use the methods that we have used to collect ours, which are standard and well-proven ones, they will have a robust data set, which they can use to compare outcomes for their children against those for all other children in Scotland, who might not have been experiencing the same delivery of support. There are a number of options.

So that alternative route could be used to inform local decisions.

Paul Bradshaw

Yes.

Paul Wheelhouse

That is helpful.

We have touched on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Is there any evidence that parents with higher educational attainment and perhaps higher income are more likely to put their children forward to be assessed for the likes of ADHD? Does that extra step that such families take lead to further divergence? Mark McDonald raised a similar point earlier.

Paul Bradshaw

Yes. We have a small amount of data that hints at that, but with the new birth cohort we are collecting better data, which will give us more of an inkling. There definitely is an indication that more-educated parents are more able to identify developmental delays and will seek help for their children earlier. Less-educated parents might be less aware of the speech or physical development stage at which their children should be at certain points.

Mark McDonald

Elaine Murray made a point about behavioural differences being spotted in children from less-advantaged backgrounds. Could some of that behaviour be due to undiagnosed ADHD or autism, for example? It might not necessarily be that the parents’ behaviour is having an impact, but that they are not putting their children forward for assessment and there are, therefore, undiagnosed disabilities.

Paul Bradshaw

Absolutely.

Paul Wheelhouse

Both you and Donna Bell have talked about messages regarding behaviour and the symptoms of problems. How big an impact could the issue have on the delivery of teaching, particularly at the early years stage? Will it help to inform us in changing the approach? I am on the parent council of my local school, and have often seen how a change of teacher can have a dramatic impact on the outcomes for some of those who are seen as problem children. A change of approach, with perhaps a fresher view and teaching style and more buy-in to modern teaching methods can have a dramatic impact on individual children’s performance. Have you picked up any key messages there?

Paul Bradshaw

We have not measured that. In our data collection, we have not asked whether there has been a change of teacher, but I agree that that could have an impact on children’s experiences at school.

Donna Bell

In pre-school environments and early learning settings in primary school, the workforce is clearly the key measure of quality. We have invested quite a bit in the workforce in those environments to ensure that it is absolutely up to date with the latest thinking in child development, modern teaching methods and engagement with children—exactly the matters that Paul Wheelhouse raised. We have done a lot of work on that in the past three to four years, and it is starting to pay dividends in the early learning environment.

Paul Bradshaw

As part of our dissemination programme, we regularly share our findings with all student teachers attending Moray house. Over the past two years, we have spoken to all bachelor of education and professional graduate diploma in education students. There has been a practical presentation on the study and on how to understand the findings, followed by workshop exercises on how the students might use the findings on a day-to-day basis when they become teachers.

We regularly attend and speak at local authority events for early years practitioners. We have spoken to practitioners in most local authorities in Scotland, so people who are dealing with children with those difficulties are aware of our findings.

That is excellent. Thank you.

12:00

John Mason

Following on from what Paul Wheelhouse said, my question is about paragraph 3.1.1 in your briefing paper, which is on cognitive ability. I am interested in whether there were changes in the three cohorts. Some of the figures that are referred to are quite stark, such as the 18-month gap in vocabulary ability by the age of three, which persists during the pre-school year. Further on, the paper talks about the importance of early communication skills “at age 22 months”, which is very young. Going on what has been said previously, if we produce more leaflets asking parents to come forward, that would tend to help the more educated ones; if anything, it might widen the gap. Might the gap widen or narrow in future?

Paul Bradshaw

We do not yet have data that allows us to look at differences in cognitive ability between the cohorts. That was not measured with the child cohort. We will look to measure it with the new birth cohort, but our first contact with them at age three will be next time round.

With the continuing focus on the importance of early years and the delivery of programmes to support development in the early years, I hope that the gap will not widen. I do not expect the gap to widen. I am not sure to what extent the gap will narrow, but I am optimistic that it will narrow in future.

Donna Bell

The committee has heard a lot of evidence about preventative spend. We cannot afford to see that gap widen any further.

The Government is doing a fair bit of work on early literacy and early communication skills. The play talk read campaign has a simple premise, yet when parents play, talk and read with their children it has an important impact. It sounds simple, but having those experiences fosters early communication skills at 22 months and when children make the transition to nursery. We also support bookbug, which runs sessions that are about parents reading and singing with their children. Bookbug is also a book gifting programme.

Those are practical applications for parents through which they can see the things that they need to do to improve their children’s skills and abilities. We are beginning to be a lot more assertive with our outreach. For example, the Scottish Book Trust will be doing an assertive outreach programme over the next three years that will go to every local authority to engage with parents who are maybe not confident about doing those things with their children and to support them to try to narrow that gap.

Are the initiatives that you are talking about largely targeted at the families that are most at risk?

Donna Bell

No. The bookbug programme is universal—it is for everybody in Scotland—although the planned assertive outreach programme will be targeted at specific areas or specific groups of parents who could do with that extra helping hand.

Marco Biagi (Edinburgh Central) (SNP)

You mentioned policy interventions. The underlying motivation for the study is to inform how we try to improve outcomes. Do you measure at an individual level whether children access these sorts of services? You mentioned looking at an area in which a service had been offered. Can you make comparisons between those who take up services and those who do not?

Paul Bradshaw

We can do that to an extent, on the basis that parents are able to tell us that they are accessing a particular service. For the past couple of sweeps with the existing birth cohort, we have asked specific questions about involvement in parenting programmes, and particularly whether families have been involved in the triple P or incredible years programmes. We have not analysed that data yet, but the intention is to gather enough information on families who access those programmes to try to draw comparisons between them.

We have a lot of information about contact with services for general advice and for information on and support with different aspects of child development, but we have only recently started collecting data on more specific parenting support programmes.

Is access to services more broadly, such as health visitors or specialist speech support, included in the data set?

Paul Bradshaw

Yes.

Marco Biagi

That is interesting.

Perhaps this is stating the obvious, but I suppose that the advantage of a large study like this is that it can find relative strength.

The direction of travel in early years policy making has been that we cannot easily do anything about the fundamental underlying wealth inequality, but we can try to provide support for parenting. How strong an effect will improved parenting have on closing the gap if the policies are successful in bringing that about? Paragraph 3.1.1 of your briefing paper states that stronger parent-child attachment, for example, is associated with improved vocabulary ability. How strongly does that close the gap?

Paul Bradshaw

That is difficult to say. Given the strength of different circumstances and characteristics, any robust measure of advantage or disadvantage, such as the level of parental education or income, will have a much more dominant effect than more minor characteristics such as parenting, so it is difficult to define the extent to which that will make a difference. What we know for certain, however, is that such characteristics can make a difference, particularly for children in disadvantaged circumstances. There are programmes that are capable of providing support for that and improving children’s experiences.

Donna Bell

A lot of the international evidence and some of the evidence from Scotland suggests that some people in financially straitened circumstances are very good parents. We would not make a link between such circumstances and poor parenting. It is very clear to us that it is about what parents do, not what they are—we just need to remember that.

I am interested in how far it closes the gap when a parent from a family that lacks resources and faces lots of challenges is able to deliver better parenting. However, you are saying that you cannot really measure that at the moment.

Paul Bradshaw

We have not looked at that specifically in the context of the data that we have gathered thus far.

Donna Bell

The feedback that we are getting in our discussions on the parenting strategy is that, although circumstances might be difficult, parents love their children and want the best for them, but circumstances can sometimes be so overwhelming that the parents do not have the capacity to provide that. In circumstances in which a lost shoe is the end of the world, it is the straw that breaks the camel’s back. An awful lot of work remains to be done to give parents the capacity to get themselves to a point at which they are able to look after their children as they want to.

The Convener

I thank our witnesses for coming along and giving evidence. I also thank members for their questions.

On 22 February, we agreed to take the next item in private. I therefore close the public part of the meeting.

12:08 Meeting continued in private until 12:56.